Dr. Yevgen Bogodistov
University
of Applied Sciences Neu-Ulm (
Moritz Botts
MANAGEMENT AND RELATIONSHIPS: ISSUES IN CONDITIONS OF
A HIGH UNCERTAINTY
Since its introduction in 1991 by anthropologist Alan
Fiske, the relational models theory (RMT) has become one of the most prominent
theories explaining human relationships[8]. This theory was introduced to social psychology in
1992, shedding light on aspects of social relations such as generation,
interpretation, coordination, and evaluation of social interactions [5]. For example, RMThas been
applied to questions of trust in institutions [3] connecting the issues of trust with a prevailing
relational model [10]. Recent research in management suggests that relational models are a moderator which explains efficiency,
team congruence, and even dynamic capabilities of organizations [1].
Relational models theory proposes four elementary
models of relationships: CS or communal sharing (family-like relationships,
where parties share common value and focus on group’s commonalities); EM or
equality matching (friendship-like relationship, where parties rely on fair
exchange and reciprocal interactions); AR or authority ranking (linear of
hierarchical relationships, where one of the parties enjoys leading positions,
whereas the other party obeys); and, MP or market pricing (relationships based
on exact calculations of invested time and effort and the according relational
interactions) [6].
The study of Bogodistov and Botts (forthcoming) [1] was made in the non-profit domain (military units)
but the results of the study might be generalizable
for the for-profit organizations since military units are to some extent
predecessors of the known management and organizational structures [11]. The researchers focused on the two relational
models that were most appropriate for the military domain, namely the
In order to find an answer to the mentioned research
question we performed a quantitative survey (questionnaire) with 70 soldiers
who at the moment of the study were actively engaged in anti-terrorist
operation (ATO) in the East of Ukraine. During the preparation of the analysis
we made a translation of the Haslam and Fiske
relational models questionnaire (Haslam and Fiske,
1999) and of NATO’s questionnaire “CTEF 2.0:
Assessment and Improvement of Command Team Effectiveness” (Essens
et al., 2010) into the Russian language. The Russian version of the
questionnaire was back translated into English and checked for consistency by a
native speaker. Both versions of the questionnaire (original and the back
translated one) showed no significant differences in meaning and were
recognized to be equal.
Our dependent variables were the
In order to make the analysis we decided to use the
ANOVA/MANOVAmethod. Prior to conducting the MANOVA,
we conducted a Box’s M value test. It produced the value of 19.017 (p = .044),
which was interpreted as non-significant based on Huberty
and Petoskey’s [9] guideline (i.e., p < .005). Thus the covariance
matrices between the groups were assumed to be equal for the purposes of the
MANOVA.A statistically significant MANOVA effect was obtained, Pillais’ Trace = .219, F (2, 61) = 8.541, p = .001 for the trust,
and Pillais’ Trace = .157, F (2, 61) = 5.688, p =.005
for the time in the ATO zone. Other variables did not produce direct
significant results. The multivariate effect size was estimated at .158 and
.157 respectively, which implies that 15.8% and 15.7% of the variance on the
canonically derived dependent variable was accounted for by trust and time
spent in ATO. Also the task instability produced no
significant effects neither directly nor as moderator, the pleasantness
played a moderating role: Pillais’ Trace = .097, F
(2, 61) = 3.283, p =.044. This finding supports our assumption that
pleasantness of the relations with the commander moderate
the relationship between trust and the preferred relational model. 9.7% of
variance in the dependent variable is explained by the interaction of
pleasantness and trust.
Prior to conducting a series of follow up ANOVAs, the
homogeneity of variance for the three relational models was tested. Based on a
series of Levene’s tests, the homogeneity of variance
assumption was considered satisfied (EM: F (3, 66) = 2.950, p = .039; AR: F (3, 66) = 1.579, p = .203). We found that low
pleasantness strongly reduces effects of trust on the preference of the EM
model (negative moderation) – B = -.679, p = .013, whereas this relationship
was not significant for the AR model. Trust, on the contrary, played a
significant and positive role for the AR model (B = .346, p = .023), whereas no
significant relationship for the EM model was found. Age played a weakly
significant role for the EM model preference (B = -.026, p = .095). Time in ATO
has a small but significant impact on the AR model preference (B = .059, p =
.001).
We interpret our findings as follows. Authority
ranking is a model which needs a linear type of relationships. This model is,
however, not always a reflection of a formal organizational structure. In many
cases, even in relationships with managers (commanders), a different type of
relational models is preferred. As has been shown elsewhere, different
relational models suit different organizational needs. Trust proved to be
important in the preference of the AR model. Probably, soldiers stick to this
model both in their formal and informal communication if they trust and feel
trusted by the commander. Otherwise they prefer other models of relationships.
EM or friendship-like fairness-based relationship are
dependent on the feelings of the soldier in the relationships. Feeling happy
positively impacts the preference for the EM model in relationships with the
manager (commander). Although we did not make any assumptions concerning the
influence of the time spent in the ATO, we would like to emphasize its role.
Probably time spent together with a commander helps in establishing trust
which, in turn, positively impacts the AR model. We would like to stress the
necessity of further research in this field.
List
of references:
1. Bogodistov Y., Botts M., 2016
(forthcoming). Dynamic capabilities in extremely dynamic
environments: Where “competitive advantage” equals “lives.” Presented
at the 76th Annual Meeting of the
2. BradleyM.M.,
LangP.J., 1994. Measuring
emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 25, 49–59.
3. Earle T. C., Cvetkovich G., 2013. Social Trust
and Culture in Risk Management, in: Cvetkovich G., LofstedtR.E. (Eds.), Social Trust and the
Management of Risk. Routledge. Earle T. C., Cvetkovich G.,
1997. Culture, cosmopolitanism, and risk management. Risk Analysis 17, 55–65.
4. EssensP.,
VogelaarA., MylleJ., BaranskiJ., GoodwinG., Van BuskirkW., BerggrenP., HofT.,
2010. CTEF 2.0–Assessment and Improvement of Command Team
Effectiveness: Verification of Model and Instrument, 121–123.
5. Fiske A. P., 1992. The four elementary
forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychol Rev 99, 689–723.
6. Fiske A. P., 1991. Structures of social
life: The four elementary forms of human relations: Communal sharing, authority
ranking, equality matching, market pricing. Free Press, 1–500.
7. Haslam N., 2004.
Relational models theory: a contemporary overview. Psychology Press, 1–373.
8. Haslam, N., Fiske, A.P., 1999. Relational models theory: a
confirmatory factor analysis. Personal Relationships 6, 241–250.
9. Huberty, C.J., Petoskey, M.D., 2000. Multivariate
analysis of variance and covariance.Handbook of
applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling, 183-208.
10. Sheppard, B.H.,
11. Talbot, P.A., 2003. Management organisational history - a
military lesson? Journal of European Industrial Training 27, 330–340.